关键词不能为空

当前您在: 主页 > 英语 >

Testing for Competence Rather Than for Intelligence(word版)

作者:高考题库网
来源:https://www.bjmy2z.cn/gaokao
2021-02-08 16:43
tags:

-

2021年2月8日发(作者:拉丁美洲)


Testing for Competence RatherThan for


The


testing


movement


in


the


United


States


hasbeen


a


success,


if


one


judges


success


by the usualAmerican criteria of size, influence, and igence


and


aptitude


tests


are


usednearly


everywhere


by


schools,


colleges,


and



is


a


sign


of


backwardness


not


to


havetest


scores


in


the


school


records


of


children.


TheEducational


Testing


Service


alone


employs


about2,000


people,


annually


administers


Scholastic


AptitudeTests


to


thousands


of


aspirants


to


college,


andmakes


enough


money


to


support


a


large


basic


researchoperation.


Its


tests


have


tremendous


powerover


the


lives


of


young


people


by


stamping


some


ofthem



and others



protest


of


some),


the


tests


have


served


asa


very


efficient


device


for


screening


out


black,Spanish-speaking,


and


other


minority


applicants


tocolleges.


Admissions


officers


have


protested


thatthey


take


other


qualities


besides


test


achievementsinto


account in granting admission, but carefulstudies by Wing and Wallach (1971) and


othershave shown that this is true only to a very limiteddegree.


Why should intelligence or aptitude tests haveall this power? What justifies the


use of such testsin selecting applicants for college entrance or jobs?On what


assumptions is the success of the movementbased? They deserve careful examination


beforewe


go


on


rather


blindly


promoting


the


use


oftests


as


instruments


of


power


over


the lives of manyAmericans.


The key issue is obviously the validity of socalledintelligence tests. Their use


could not bejustified unless they were valid, and it is myconviction that the


evidence for their validity is byno means so overwhelming as most of us, rather


unthinkingly,had come to think it was. In point offact, most of us just believed


the results that thetesters gave us, without subjecting them to thekind of fierce


skepticism that greets, for example,the latest attempt to show that ESP exists.


Myobjectives are to review skeptically the main lines ofevidence for the validity


of intelligence and aptitudetests and to draw some inferences from this reviewas


to new lines that testing might take in the future.


Let us grant at the outset that brain-damagedor retarded people do less well on


intelligence


teststhan


other


people.


Wechsler


(19S8)


initially


usedthis


criterion


to


validate


his


instrument,


although


ithas


an


obvious


weakness:


brain-damaged


people


doless


well


on


almost


any


test


so


that


it


is


hard


toargue


that


something


unique


called



of


intelligence


responsible


for


the


deficiency


in


testscores.


The


multimethod, multitrait criterion hasnot been applied here.


Tests Predict Grades in School



The games people are required to play on aptitudetests are similar to the games


teachers


requirein


the


classroom.


In


fact,


many


of


Binet's


originaltests


were


taken


from exercises that teachers usedin French schools. So it is scarcely surprising


thataptitude test scores are correlated highly withgrades in school. The whole


Scholastic


AptitudeTesting


movement


rests


its


case


largely


on


thissingle


undeniable


fact.


Defenders


of


intelligencetesting,


like


McNemar


(1964),


often


seem


to


besuggesting


that


this


is


the


only


kind


of


r


remarked


that



manualof


the


Differential


Aptitude


Test


of


the


PsychologicalCorporation


contains a staggering total of 4,096,yes I counted 'em, validity coefficients.


Whatmore could you ask for, ladies and gentlemen? Itwas not until I looked at the


manual


myself


(Mc-Nemar


certainly


did


not


enlighten


me)


that


I


confirmedmy


suspicion


that almost every one of those


incourses



in other words, performing on similar typesof tests.


So


what


about


grades?


How


valid


are


they


aspredictors?


Researchers


have


in


fact


had


greatdifficulty


demonstrating


that


grades


in


school


arerelated


to


any


other


behaviors of importance



other than doing well on aptitude tests. Yet thegeneral


public



including many psychologists andmost college officials



simply has been


unable


tobelieve


or


accept


this


fact.


It


seems


so


self-evidentto


educators


that


those


who


do


well


in


their


classesmust


go


on


to


do


better


in


life


that


they


systematicallyhave


disregarded


evidence


to


the


contrary


thathas


been


accumulating


for


some


time.


In


theearly


1950s,


a


committee


of


the


Social


Science


ResearchCouncil


of


which


I


was


chairman


looked


intothe


matter


and


concluded


that


while


grade


level


attainedseemed related to future measures of successin life, performance within


grade was related onlyslightly. In other words, being a high school orcollege


graduate gave one a credential that openedup certain higher level jobs, but the


poorer studentsin high school or college did as well in life as thetop students.


As a college teacher, I found thishard to believe until I made a simple check. I


tookthe


top


eight


students


in


a


class


in


the


late


1940sat


Wesleyan


University


where


I was teaching



allstraight A students



and contrasted what theywere doing in the


early


1960s


with


what


eightreally


poor


students


were


doing



all


of


whom


weregetting


barely


passing


averages


in


college


(C




orbelow).


To


my


great


surprise,


I


could


not


distinguishthe two lists of men 15-18 years were lawyers, doctors,


research scientists, andcollege and high school teachers in both only


difference


I


noted


was


that


those


withbetter


grades


got


into


better


law


or


medical


schools,but


even


with


this


supposed


advantage


they


did


nothave


notably


more


successful


careers


as


comparedwith


the


poorer


students


who


had


had


to


be


satisfiedwith



law


and


medical


schools


at


theoutset.


Doubtless


the


C




students


could


not


getinto


even


second-rate


law


and


medical


schools


underthe


stricter admissions testing standards of that an advantage for society?


Such outcomes have been documented carefullyby many researchers (cf. Hoyt, 1965)


both inBritain (Hudson, 1960) and in


the United (1970), in a book


suggestively titled Educationand Jobs: The Great Training Robbery, hassummarized


studies showing that neither amountof education nor grades in school are related


to vocationalsuccess as a factory worker, bank teller, orair traffic controller.


Even


for


highly


intellectualjobs


like


scientific


researcher,


Taylor,


Smith,


andGhiselin (1963) have shown that superior on-thejobperformance is related in no


way to bettergrades in college. The average college grade forthe top third in


research


success


was


2.73


(aboutB




)


,


and


for


the


bottom


third,


2.69


(also


B-).Such


facts


have


been


known


for


some


time.


Theymake


it


abundantly


clear


that


the


testing


movementis


in


grave


danger


of


perpetuating


a


mythologicalmeritocracy


in


which


none


of


the


measures


of


meritbears


significant


demonstrable


validity


with


respectto


any


measures outside of the charmed logists used to say as a kind of an



jokethat


intelligence


is


what


the


intelligence


tests



seems


to


be


uncomfortably


near


thewhole


truth


and


nothing


but


the


truth.


But


what'sfunny


about


it, when the public took us moreseriously than we did ourselves and used the tests


toscreen


people


out


of


opportunities


for


educationand


high-status


jobs?


And


why


call


excellence atthese test games intelligence?


Even further, why keep the best education forthose who are already doing well at


the


games?This


in


effect


is


what


the


colleges


are


doing


whenthey


select


from


their


applicants those with thehighest Scholastic Aptitude Test scores. Isn't thislike


saying


that


we


will


coach


especially


those


whoalready


can


play


tennis


well?


One


would


thinkthat


the


purpose


of


education


is


precisely


to


improvethe


performance


of


those


who


are


not


doingvery


well.


So


when


psychologists


predict


on


thebasis


of


the


Scholastic


Aptitude


Test


who


is


mostlikely


to


do


well


in


college,


they


are


suggestingimplicitly


that


these


are


the



bets


to



in


another


sense,


if


the


colleges


were


interestedin


proving


that


they


could


educate


people,


highscoringstudents might be poor bets because theywould be less likely to show


improvement in be sure, the teachers want studentswho will do well


in their courses, but should societyallow the teachers to determine who deserves


to beeducated, particularly when the performance of interestto teachers bears so


little relation to anyother type of life performance?


Do Intelligence Tests Tap Abilities ThatAre Responsible for Job Success?


Most


psychologists


think


so;


certainly


the


generalpublic


thinks


so


(Cronbach,


1970,


p. 300), butthe evidence is a whole lot less satisfactory thanone would think it


ought to be to justify suchconfidence.


Thorndike


and


Hagen


(19S9),


for


instance,


obtained12,000


correlations


between


aptitude


testscores


and


various


measures


of


later


occupationalsuccess


on


over


10,000


respondents


and


concludedthat


the


number


of


significant


correlations


did


notexceed


what


would


be


expected


by


chance.


Inother


words,


the


tests


were


invalid.


Yet


psychologistsgo on using them, trusting that the poorvalidities must be due to


restriction in range dueto the fact that occupations do not admit individualswith


lower


scores.


But


even


here


it


is


notclear


whether


the


characteristics


required


for


entryare,


in


fact,


essential


to


success


in


the


field.


Onemight


suppose


that


finger


dexterity


is


essential


tobeing


a


dentist,


and


require


a


minimum


test


scorefor


entry.


Yet,


it


was


found


by


Thorndike


andHagen


(1959)


to


be


related


negatively


to


incomeas


a dentist! Holland and Richards (1965) andElton and Shevel (1969) have shown that


no


consistentrelationships


exist


between


scholastic


aptitudescores


in


college


students


and


their


actualaccomplishments


in


social


leadership,


the


arts,


science,music, writing, and speech and drama.


Yet what are we to make of Ghiselli's (1966, p.121) conclusions, based on a review


of


50


years


ofresearch,


that


general


intelligence


tests


correlate


.42with


trainability


and .23


with


proficiency


across


alltypes


of


jobs?


Each


of


these


correlations is basedon over 10,000 cases. It is small wonder thatpsychologists


believe intelligence tests are validpredictors of job success. Unfortunately, it


is


impossibleto


evaluate


Ghiselli's


conclusion,


as


he


doesnot


cite


his


sources


and


he


does


not


state


exactlyhow


job


proficiency


was


measured


for


each


of


hiscorrelations.


We can draw some conclusions fromhis results, however, and we can make a good


guessthat job proficiency often was measured by supervisors'ratings or by such


indirect


indicators


ofsupervisors'


opinions


as


turnover,


promotion,


salaryincreases, and the like.


What


is


interesting


to


observe


is


that


intelligencetest


correlations


with


proficiency


in


higher


statusjobs


are


regularly


higher


than


with


proficiency


inlower


status jobs (Ghiselli, 1966, pp. 34, 78).Consider the fact that intelligence test


scores correlate



.08 with proficiency as a canvasser orsolicitor and .45 with


proficiency as a stock andbond salesman. This should be a strong clue as towhat


intelligence tests are getting at, but most observershave overlooked it or simply


assumed thatit takes more general ability to be a stock andbond salesman than a


canvasser.


But


these


twojobs


differ


also


in


social


status,


in


the


language,accent,


clothing, manner, and connections by educationand family necessary for success in


the



basic


problem


with


many


job


proficiencymeasures


for


validating


ability


tests is that theydepend heavily on the credentials the man brings tothe job



the


habits, values, accent, interests, etc.



that mean he is acceptable to management


and


toclients.


Since


we


also


know


that


social


class


backgroundis


related


to


getting


higher ability test scores(Nuttall&Fozard, 1970), as well as to having theright


personal


credentials


for


success,


the


correlationbetween


intelligence


test


scores


and


job


successoften


may


be


an


artifact,


the


product


of


theirjoint


association


with


class


status.


Employers


mayhave


a


right


to


select


bond


salesmen


who


have


goneto


the


right


schools


because


they


do


better,


butpsychologists


do


not


have


a


right


to


argue


that itis their intelligence that makes them more proficientin their jobs.


We know that correlation does not equal causation,but we keep forgetting it. Far


too


many


psychologistsstill


report


average-ability


test


scores


forhigh-


and


low- prestige occupations, inferring incorrectlythat this evidence shows it takes


more


of


thistype


of


brains


to


perform


a


high-level


than


a


lowleveljob.


For


instance,


Jensen (1972) wroterecently:


He


certainly


leaves


the


impression


that


it


is



as


we


ordinarily


think


of it


prestige. But the association canbe interpreted as meaning, just as reasonably,


thatit takes more -pull, more opportunity, to get thevocabulary and other habits


required


by


those


inpower


from


incumbents


of


high-status


l


studies


that try to separate the credentialfactor from the ability factor in job success


havebeen very few in number.


Ghiselli (1966) simply did not deal with theproblem of what the criteria of job


proficiency


maymean


for


validating


the


tests.


For


example,


he


reporteda


correlation


of .27


between


intelligence


testscores


and


proficiency


as


a


policeman


or


a


detective(p. 83), with no attention given to the very importantissues involved in


how a policeman's performanceis to be evaluated. Will supervisors' ratingsdo? If


so,


it


discriminates


against


blackpolicemen


(Baehr,


Furcon,


&Froemel,


1968)


becausewhite


supervisors


regard


them


as



what


about


the


public?


Shouldn't


their


opinionas


to


how


they


are


served


by


the


police


be


partof


the


criterion?


The


most


recent


careful


review(Kent


&


Eisenberg,


1972)


of


the


evidence


relatingability


test


scores


to


police


performance


concludedthat


there


is


no


stable,


significant



is


concrete


evidence


that


one


must


view


withconsiderable skepticism the assumed relation of intelligencetest scores to


success on the job.


One other illustration may serve to warn theunwary about accepting uncritically


simple statementsabout the role of ability, as measured byintelligence tests, in


life


outcomes.


It


is


statedwidely


that


intelligence


promotes


general


adjustmentand


results in lower neuroticism. For example,Anderson (1960) reported a significant


correlationbetween intelligence test scores obtainedfrom boys in 1950, age 14-17,


and


follow-up


ratingsof


general


adjustment


made


five


years


later.


Canwe


assume


that


intelligence


promotes


better


adjustmentto


life


as


has


been


often


claimed?


Itsounds


reasonable


until


we


reflect


that


the



is


a


test


of


ability


to


do


well


in


school(to


take


academic


type


tests),


that


many


of


Anderson'ssample


were


still


in school or getting startedon careers, and that those who are not doing well


in


school


or


getting


a


good


first


job


because


of


itare


likely


to


be


considered


poorly


adjusted


by


themselvesand


others.


Here


the


test


has


become


partof


the


criterion


and


has


introduced


the


correlationartificially.


In


case


this


sounds


like


special


reasoning,consider


the


fact,


not


commented


on


particularlyby


Anderson,


that


the


same


correlation


between


test


scores


and


adjustment


ingirls


was


an


insignificant


.06.


Are


we


to


concludethat


intelligence


does


not


promote


adjustmentin


girls? It would seem more reasonable to arguethat the particular ability tested,


here associatedwith scholastic success, is more important to success(and hence


adjustment)


for


boys


than


for



this


is


a


far


cry


from


the


careless


inference


thatintelligence


tests


tap


a


general


ability


to


adapt


successfullyto


life's


problems


because high-IQ children(read


To


make


the


point


even


more


vividly,


supposeyou


are


a


ghetto


resident


in


the


Roxbury


section


ofBoston.


To


qualify


for


being


a


policeman


you


haveto


take


a


three-hour-long


general intelligence testin which you must know the meaning of words like



playanalogy


games


with


them,


you


do


not


qualify andmust


be


satisfied


with


some


such


job as being ajanitor for which an


Massachusetts


Civil


Service


,


not


unreasonably,


feel


angry,


upset,and


unsuccessful. Because you do not know thosewords, you are considered to have low


intelligence,and


since


you


consequently


have


to


take


a


lowstatusjob


and


are


unhappy,


you


contribute


to


thecelebrated


correlations


of


low


intelligence


with


lowoccupational


status


and


poor


adjustment.


Psychologistsshould


be


ashamed


of


themselves for promotinga view of general intelligence that has encouragedsuch a


testing program, particularly whenthere is no solid evidence that significantly


relatesperformance


on


this


type


of


intelligence


test


withperformance


as


a


policeman.


The Role oj Power in Controlling Life-Outcome Criteria


Psychologists


have


been,


until


recently,


incrediblynaive


about


the


role


of


powerful


interests in controllingthe criteria against which psychologistshave validated


their


tests.


Terman


felt


that


hisstudies


had


proved


conclusively


that



success. By and large, psychologistshave agreed with him. Kohlberg, LaCrosse,and


Ricks


(1970),


for


instance,


in


a


recent


summarystatement concluded


that


Terman


and


Oden's(1947)


study



the


gifted


were


more


successfuloccupationally,


maritally, and socially thanthe average group, and were lower in 'morallydeviant'


forms


of


psychopathology


(e.g.,

< p>
alcoholism,homosexuality).


Jensen


(1972)


agreed:


One


of


the


most


convincing


demonstrations


that


I.Q.


isrelated


to



life


indicators of ability was provided ina classic study by Terman and his associates


at StanfordUniversity. . . . Terman found that for the most partthese high-I.Q.


children


in


later


adulthood


markedly


excelledthe


general


population


on


every


indicator


of


achievementthat


was


examined:


a


higher


level


of


education


completed;more scholastic honors and awards; higher occupationalstatus; higher


income; production of more articles,books, patents and other signs of creativity;


more


entriesin


Who's


Who;


a


lower


mortality


rate;


better


physical


andmental


health;


and


a


lower


divorce


rate.


.


.


.


Findings


suchas


these


establish


beyond


a


doubt


that


I.Q.


tests


measurecharacteristics


that


are


obviously


of


considerable


importancein


our present technological society. To say that the kindof ability measured by


intelligence tests is irrelevant orunimportant would be tantamount to repudiating


civilizationas we know it [p. 9],



I


do


not


want


to


repudiate


civilization


as


we


knowit,


or


even


to


dismiss


intelligence


tests


as


irrelevantor


unimportant,


but


I


do


want


to


state,


as


emphaticallyas


possible,


that


Terman's


studies


do


notdemonstrate


unequivocally


that


it


is


the


kind


ofability


measured


by


the


intelligence


tests


that


isresponsible


for


(i.e.,


causes)


the


greater


success ofthe high- IQ children. Terman's studies may showonly that the rich and


powerful


have


more


opportunities,and


therefore


do


better


in


life.


And


if


thatis


even


possibly


true,


it


is


socially


irresponsible


tostate


that


psychologists


have


established



adoubt


that


the


kind


of


ability


measured


by


intelligencetests


is


essential


for


high-level


performancein


our


society.


For,


by


current


methodologicalstandards, Terman's studies (and others like them)were naive. No


attempt


was


made


to


equate


foropportunity


to


be


successful


occupationally


andsocially.


His


gifted


people


clearly


came


fromsuperior


socioeconomic


backgrounds


to those hecompared them with (at one point all men in California,including day


laborers). He had no unequivocalevidence that it was


his


test


scores)


that


was


responsible


forthe


superior


performance


of


his


group.


It


would beas legitimate (though also not proven) to concludethat sons of the rich,


powerful,


and


educated


wereapt


to


be


more


successful


occupationally,


maritally,and


socially


because


they


had


more


material



make


the


point


in


another


way,


considerthe


data


in


Table


1,


which


are


fairly


representativeof


findings


in


this


area.


They were obtainedby Havighurst, Bowman, Liddle, Matthews,and Pierce (1962) from


a


typical


town


in


MiddleAmerica.


One


observes


the


usual


strong


relationshipbetween


social


class


and


IQ


and


between


IQand


college- going



which


leads


on


to


occupationalsuccess. The traditional interpretation of suchfindings is that more


stupid


children


come


from


thelower


classes


because


their


parents


are


also


stupidwhich


explains


why


they


are


lower


class.


A


higherproportion


of


children


with


high IQ go to collegebecause they are more intelligent and more suitedto college


study. This is as it should be becauseIQ predicts academic success. The fact that


moreintelligent


people


going


to


college


come


more


oftenfrom


the


upper


class


follows


naturally


because


theupper


classes


contain


more


intelligent


people.


Sothe


traditional argument has gone for years. Itseemed all very simple and obvious to


Terman andhis followers.


However, a closer look at Table 1 suggests anotherinterpretation that is equally


plausible,


thoughnot


more


required


by


the


data


than


the


one


justgiven.


Compare


the


percentages going to collegein the



high socioeconomic statusand


low IQ versus high IQ and low socioeconomicstatus. It appears to be no more likely


for thebright children (high IQ) from the lower classes togo to college (despite


their high aptitude for it)than for the


Why


is


this?


An


obvious


possibility


isthat


the


bright


but


poor


children


do


not


have


themoney


to


go


to


college,


or


they


do


not


want


to


go,preferring


to


work


or


do


other


things. In the currentlingo, they are


not


had


access


to


the


other


factors(values,


aspirations,


money)


that


promote


collegegoingin upper-class children. But now we havean alternative explanation of


college- going



namely,socioeconomic


status


which


seems


to


be


as


good


apredictor


of


this


type


of


success


as


ability.


Howcan


we


claim


that


ability


as


measured


by


thesetests


is


the


critical


factor


in


college-going?


Veryfew


children,


even


with


good


test-taking ability, goto college if they are from poor families. One couldargue


that


they


are


victims


of


oppression:


theydo


not


have


the


opportunity


or


the


values


thatpermit or encourage going to college. Isn't it likelythat the same oppressive


forces may have preventedeven more of them from learning to playschool games well


at all?


Belonging


to


the


power


elite


(high


socioeconomicstatus)


not


only


helps


a


young


man


go to collegeand get jobs through contacts his family has, italso gives him easy


access


as


a


child


to


the


credentialsthat


permit


him


to


get


into


certain


ys,


those


credentials


include


thewords


and


word-game


skills


used


in


Scholastic


AptitudeTests.


In


the


Middle


Ages


they


requiredknowledge


of


Latin


for


the


learned


professions


oflaw,


medicine,


and


theology.


Only


those


youngmen


who


could


read and write Latin could get intothose occupations, and if tests had been given


inLatin, I am sure they would have shown that professionalsscored higher in Latin


than


men


in


general,that


sons


who


grew


up


in


families


where


Latinwas


used


would


have


an


advantage


in


those


testscompared


to


those


in


poor


families


where


Latin


wasunknown,


and


that


these


men


were


more


likely


toget


into


the


professions.


But


would


we


concludewe were dealing with a general ability factor?Many a ghetto resident must


or should feel that heis in a similar position with regard to the kind ofEnglish


he must learn in order to do well on tests,in school, and in occupations today in


America. Iwas recently in Jamaica where all around me poorpeople were speaking an


English that was almostentirely incomprehensible to me. If I insisted, theywould


speak


patiently


in


a


way


that


I


could


understand,but


I


felt


like


a


slow-witted


child.


I havewondered how well I would do in Jamaican societyif this kind of English were


standard among therich and powerful (which, by the way, it is not),and therefore


required


by


them


for


admission


intotheir


better


schools


and


occupations


(as


determinedby a test administered perhaps by the JamaicanTesting Service). I would


feel


oppressed,


not


lessintelligent,


as


the


test


would


doubtless


decide


I


wasbecause


I was so slow of comprehension and soignorant of ordinary vocabulary.


When


Cronbach


(1970)


concluded


that


such


atest



giving


realistic


information


on


the presenceof a handicap,


recognize


that


it


is


those


in


powerin


a


society


who


often


decide


what


is


a



should be a lot more cautious about acceptingas ultimate criteria of ability the


standards imposedby whatever group happens to be in power.


Does this mean that intelligence tests are invalid?As so often when you examine a


questioncarefully


in


psychology,


the


answer


depends


onwhat


you


mean.


Valid


for


what?


Certainly theyare valid for predicting who will get ahead in anumber of prestige


jobs


where


credentials


are



is


white


skin:


it


too


is


a


valid


predictorof


job success in prestige jobs. But no one wouldargue that white skin per se is an


ability of the celebrated correlations between so- calledintelligence


test


scores


and


success


can


lay


nogreater


claim


to


representing


an


ability


factor.



Valid


for


predicting


success


in


school?Certainly,because


school


success


depends


on


taking


similartypes


of


tests.


Yet,


neither


the


tests


nor


schoolgrades


seem


to


have


much


power


to


predict


realcompetence


in


many


life


outcomes,


aside


from


theadvantages


that credentials convey on the individualsconcerned.


Are


there


no


studies


which


show


that


general


intelligencetest


scores


predict


competence


with


all


ofthese


other


factors


controlled?


I


can


only


assertthat


I


have


had


a


very


hard


time


finding


a


goodcarefully


controlled


study


of


the


problem


becausetesters simply have not worked very hard on it:they have believed so much


that


they


were


measuringtrue


competence


that


they


have


not


botheredto


try


to


prove


that


they


were.


Studies


do


exist,of


course,


which


show


significant


positive


correlationsbetween


special


test


scores


and


job-relatedskills.


For


example,


perceptual


speed


scores


arerelated


to


clerical


proficiency.


So


are


tests


ofvocabulary, immediate memory, substitution, andarithmetic. Motor ability test


scores


are


relatedto


proficiency


as


a


vehicle


operator


(Ghiselli,


1966).And


so


on.


Here we are on the safe and uncontroversialground of using tests as criterion



this


is


a


far


cry


from


inferring


that


there


is


ageneral


ability


factor


that


enables


a


person


to


bemore


competent


in


anything


he


tries.


The


evidencefor


this


general ability factor turns out to be contaminatedheavily by the power of those


at the topof the social hierarchy to insist that the skills theyhave are the ones


that indicate superior adaptivecapacity.


Where Do We Go from Here?


Criticisms


of


the


testing


movement


are


not



Social


Science


Research


Council


Committee


onEarly


Identification


of


Talent


made


some


of


thesesame


points


nearly


IS


years


ago


(McClelland,Baldwin,


Bronfenbrenner,


&Strodtbeck,


19S8).But


the


beliefs


on


which


the


movement


is


basedare


held


so


firmly


that


such


theoretical


or


empiricalobjections


have


had


little


impact


up


to


now.


Thetesting


movement


continues


to grow and extend intoevery corner of our society. It is unlikely that itcan be


simply stopped, although minority groupsmay have the political power to stop it.


For thetests are clearly discriminatory against those whohave not been exposed to


the


culture,


entrance


towhich


is


guarded


by


the


tests.


What


hopefully


canhappen


is


that


testers


will


recognize


what


is


goingon


and


attempt


to


redirect


their


energies


in


asounder


direction.


The


report


of


the


special


committeeon


testing


to


the


College


Entrance ExaminationBoard (1970) is an important sign that changesin thinking are


occurring



if only they can be implementedat a practical level. The report's gist


isthat


a


wider


array


of


talents


should


be


assessed


forcollege


entrance


and


reported


as


a


profile


to


thecolleges.


This


is


a


step


in


the


right


direction


ifeveryone


keeps


firmly


in


mind


that


the


criteria


forestablishing


the



of


these


new


measuresreally


ought


to


be


not


grades


in


school,


but



life


in


the


broadest


theoretical and practicalsense.


But


now


I


am


on


the


spot.


Having


criticizedwhat


the


testing


movement


has


been


doing,


I


feelsome


obligation


to


suggest


alternatives.


How


wouldI


do


things


differently


or


better? I do not mindmaking suggestions, but I am well aware that someof them are


as


open


to


criticism


on


other


groundsas


the


procedures


I


have


been


criticizing.


So


Imust offer them in a spirit of considerable humility,as approaches that at least


some


people


might


beinterested


in


pursuing


who


are


discouraged


withwhat


we


have


been


doing.


My


goal


is


to


brainstorma


bit


on


how


things


might


be


different,


notto


present


hard


evidence


that


my


proposals


arebetter


than


what


has


been


done


to


date.


Howwould


one


test


for


competence,


if


I


may


use


thatword


as


a


symbol


for


an


alternative


approach


totraditional intelligence testing?


1. The best testing is criterion sampling. Thepoint is so obvious that it would


scarcely be worthmentioning, if it had not been obscured so oftenby psychologists


like McNemar and Jensen whotout a general intelligence factor. If you want toknow


how well a person can drive a car (the criterion),sample his ability to do so by


giving hima driver's test. Do not give him a paper-and-penciltest for following


directions,


a


general


intelligencetest,


etc.


As


noted


above,


there


is


ample


evidencethat tests which sample job skills will predict proficiencyon the job.


Academic


skill


tests


are


successful


precisely


becausethey


involve


criterion


sampling


for


the


mostpart.


As


already


pointed


out,


the


Scholastic


AptitudeTest


taps


skills


that


the


teacher


is


lookingfor


and


will


give


high


grades


for.


No


one


couldobject


if


it


had


been


recognized


widely


that


thiswas


all


that


was


going


on


when


aptitude


tests


wereused


to


predict


who


would


do


well


in


e


started


only


when people assumed thatthese skills had some more general validity, as impliedin


the


use


of


words


like


intelligence.


Yet,even


a


little


criterion


analysis


would


show


thatthere are almost no occupations or life situationsthat require a person to do


word


analogies,


choosethe


most


correct


of


four


alternative


meanings


ofa


word,


etc.


Criterion


sampling


means


that


testers


have


got


toget


out


of


their


offices


where


they


play endless wordand paper-and-pencil games and into the field wherethey actually


analyze


performance


into


its



you


want


to


test


who


will


be


a


goodpoliceman,


go


find


out


what


a


policeman


does.


Followhim


around,


make


a


list


of


his activities, andsample from that list in screening applicants. Someof the job


sampling


will


have


to


be


based


on


theoryas


well


as


practice.


If


policemen


generally


discriminateagainst blacks, that is clearly not part ofthe criterion because the


law says that they mustnot. So include a test which shows the applicantdoes not

-


-


-


-


-


-


-


-



本文更新与2021-02-08 16:43,由作者提供,不代表本网站立场,转载请注明出处:https://www.bjmy2z.cn/gaokao/615673.html

Testing for Competence Rather Than for Intelligence(word版)的相关文章